Jump to content
  • The Computer Audiophile
    The Computer Audiophile

    My Lying Ears

    oop.jpg

    1-Pixel.png

    As a diehard card-carrying audiophile I am interested in all things related to this wonderful hobby. I've published articles based solely on my subjective listening experience and I've published articles detailing only objective measurements and facts about products. I enjoy publishing and reading articles that cover the gamut. I also think it's healthy and interesting to be open to perspectives completely incongruent with our own. With this in mind, I was recently sent a link to the JRiver forum to read a post about one person's perspective and experience as an inquisitive listener. I really liked what I read, in the sense that it's a real world story to which many people can probably relate and it was written in a non-confrontational way. In fact every audiophile I know, golden-eared or not, has at one time or another experienced something very similar to the follow story. I'm not pushing any agenda or endorsing a point of view by publishing this article. I simply think a worthwhile read for all who enjoy this hobby as much as I do.

     

    Here is a a re-written, more complete version of the post, sent to me for publication by the author Michael.[PRBREAK][/PRBREAK]

     

     

    Recently on the Jriver forums (Link) a forum regular was describing his experience at an audio shootout where three bit perfect players were compared. Jriver had not done particularly well in the tests (only receiving 4 out of 38 votes), and there was some discussion of why that might've been the case, given that all three players were (at least notionally) bit-perfect. There were some questions about the test methodology (you can see all the gory details in the linked thread), and some good discussion about how bit-perfect players might or might not conceivably sound different. Ultimately several forum members were of the opinion that the test was basically invalid, while others thought that surely, because so many people heard a difference that there must've been a real difference to be heard.

     

    My own view on this issue is complex. I will confess that I have occasionally heard differences between bit-perfect players. But I don't believe that bit-perfect players actually sound different. That may sound like a paradox, so I'll follow it up with a second one: I don't trust my own ears to correctly detect those kinds of differences in audio. You might well ask “Why not?” Let me offer an embarrassing personal anecdote to explain my point of view about listening tests and the fallibility of the ear:

Several years ago I built a pair of home-made bi-amped speakers. They're each the size of a large washing machine and they took me the better part of a year to build (more than a month of Sundays). Because they were entirely home-made and I was trying to do an active crossover from scratch, even after they were structurally complete, they still required quite a bit of tweaking to get the crossovers dialed in and the EQ set. 

So I started by just dialing in the EQ that seemed to make sense based on the specifications of the drivers, and taking a couple of quick RTAs with pink noise. That sounded alright, and all of my friends (several of whom are musicians and/or “sound guys”) dutifully told me how great they sounded. There was just one hitch: I kept getting headaches whenever listening to the speakers, and the headaches would go away right after I turned them off. So I tried to solve the problem by tweaking some frequencies with EQ. After some tweaks, I'd think I'd made some progress (it sounded better!), and everyone who heard the changes thought the new EQ sounded better.

     

    Eventually, I even started dutifully "blindly" A/Bing new EQ with the old EQ (I'd switch between them during playback without telling my guests what I was switching, which isn't really blind at all), and my guests would invariably swear the new EQ sounded better. And I kept going with this "tuning by ear" method, often reversing previous decisions, backing and forthing and adding more and more convoluted filters. 

The most embarrassing moment (and something of a turning point) was when I was A/Bing a filter, and a friend and I were convinced we were on to something really excellent. After ten minutes of this, we realized that the filter bank as a whole was disabled. I had been toggling the individual filter, but the bank of filters wasn't on, so it wasn't actually even affecting playback at all. And we had been very convinced we heard a difference. And the headaches never went away.

Eventually the headaches (and a growing skepticism) prompted me to stop screwing around and take some real log sweep measurements (at the suggestion of one my more empirically-minded friends). Once I did, I realized that there was apparently a huge (10+ dB) semi-ultrasonic resonant peak at 18.5KHz that I couldn't even actually hear. So I fixed it and verified the fix with measurements. And then my headaches went away. 

This prompted me to take an agonizing look at the rest of the measurements and noticed that my "tuning by ear" which I (and my friends) all felt was clearly superior had turned the frequency response into a staggering sawtooth. So I systematically removed the EQ that was pushing things away from "flat," and kept the EQ that contributed to flatness, and re-verified with measurements. The result sounded so different, and so much more natural that I was embarrassed to have wasted months messing around trying to use my "golden ears" to tune my speakers. And my wife (who had been encouraging, but politely non-committal about my EQ adventure) came home and asked unprompted if I had done something different with the speakers, and said they sounded much better. And she was right; they did. In a few afternoons, I had done more to move things forward than I had in months of paddling around. 


     

    The point of this anecdote is not to try and prove to anyone that my measurement-derived EQ sounded better than my ear-derived EQ or that a flat frequency response will sound best: as it happens, I ultimately preferred a frequency slope that isn't perfectly flat, but I couldn't even get that far by ear. 

The point is that taking actual measurements had allowed me to:


     

    1) Cure my ultrasonic frequency-induced headaches;


    2) Improve the fidelity of my system (in the literal sense of audio fidelity as "faithfulness to the source"); and


    3) Ultimately find the EQ curve that I liked best (which looked nothing like my ear-tuned curve).



     

    My ears (and the inadvertently biased ears of my friends) did not allow me to do any of those things, and in fact led me far astray on issue 2). My ears couldn't even really get me to 3) because I kept reversing myself and getting tangled up in incremental changes. Most damning, my ears were not even reliably capable of detecting no change if I thought there was a change to be heard. 

Once I realized all this, it was still surprisingly hard to admit that I had been fooling myself, and that I was so easily fooled! So I have sympathy for other people who don't want to believe that their own ears may be unreliable, and I understand why folks get mad at any suggestion that their perception may be fallible. I've been accused by many indignant audiophiles of having a tin ear, and if I could only hear what they hear, then I'd be immediately persuaded. But my problem is not that I am unpersuaded: it's that I'm too easily persuaded! I'll concede, of course, that it's possible that I have tin ears and other people's ears are much more reliable than mine, but the literature concerning the placebo effect, expectation bias, and confirmation bias in scientific studies suggests that I'm probably not entirely alone. 

And I've seen the exact same phenomenon played out with other people (often very bright people with very good ears) enough times that I find it embarrassing to watch sighted listening tests of any kind because they are so rarely conducted in a way designed to produce any meaningful information and lead into dark serpentines of false information and conclusions. 



     

    

So to bring things back around: if some bit perfect audio players have devised a way to improve their sound they have presumably done so through careful testing, in which case they should be able to provide measurements (whether distortion measurements on an analog output, digital loopback measurements, measurements of the data stream going to the DAC, or something) that validates that claim. If they claim that their output "sounds better" but does not actually measure better using current standards of measurement, they should be able to at least articulate a hypothetical measurement that would show their superiority. If they claim that the advantage isn't measurable, or that you should "just trust your ears" than they are either fooling themselves or you.

In a well-established field of engineering in which a great deal of research and development has been done, and in which there is a mature, thriving commercial market, one generally does not stumble blindly into mysterious gains in performance. Once upon a time you could discover penicillin by accident, or build an automobile engine at home. But you do not get to the moon, cure cancer, or improve a modern car's fuel efficiency by inexplicable accident. In an era where cheap-o motherboard DACs have better SNR's than the best studio equipment from 30 years ago, you don't improve audio performance by inexplicable accident either. If someone has engineered a "better than bit perfect" player they should be able to prove it, as they likely did their own testing as part of the design process. If they can't rigorously explain why (or haven't measured their own product!), let them at least explain what they have done in a way that is susceptible of proof and repetition. Otherwise what they are selling is not penicillin, it's patent medicine. 

Bottom line: if you and a group of other people hear a difference, there may really be a difference, but there may not be too. Measurements are the easy way to find out if there is really a difference. Once you've actually established that there is a real, measurable difference, only then does it make sense to do a properly conducted listening test to determine if that difference is audible. Otherwise you're just eating random mold to find out if it will help your cough (or headache, as the case may be).

     

    Or you can do what I do for the most part these days: just relax and enjoy the music.

     

     

    - Michael

     

     

     

    1-Pixel.png




    User Feedback

    Recommended Comments



    That's actually not correct.

     

    A/B/X is not the only way to do double-blinded tests, and it may be far from the best way, depending on what you are testing for.

     

    Thanks for pointing that out. There are many ways to perform blinded testing. IMO SBT styled testing is very doable for audio.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Sure. On the flip side, anything that would make the mic less effective would be a concern, and similarly for anything that would affect the ability of the human listener to discriminate.

     

    I hope you aren't championing the position that any form of discriminate testing would fall into scope of "affect the ability of the human listener to discriminate".

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    I hope you aren't championing the position that any form of discriminate testing would fall into scope of "affect the ability of the human listener to discriminate".

     

    Nope. I'm thinking you're probably familiar enough with my posts by now that you know I don't like sweeping generalizations like that.

     

    As one example of something I'd trust people to discriminate, I think people are great at discriminating volume, to the point that people will prefer a sample so slightly louder they don't even consciously realize it. Conversely, for the same reason, I would not trust people to discriminate between two samples unless they were *very* closely volume matched.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Lots of references here: The Well-Tempered Computer

    Hadn't looked at the welltemperedcomputer link in a while, thanks - there's lots of good info there

    I followed the link to Nwavguy blog & one thing occurs to me - he shows a number of jitter measurements for different devices & I can't see much distortion above -100dB (except for some low frequency jitter).

     

    The thing is - what is the "accepted" audibility threshold for small distortions?

     

    This thread on Head-fi is a good read when Ultmusicsnob starts to post & actually do some Foobar ABX tests on different music files that have various amounts of correlated & uncorrelated jitter added

     

    Through the thread he does ABX tests on files which have less & less jitter. Here's one of his later ABX tests Jitter Correlation to Audibility - Page 13

     

    And there are a number of interesting aspects to this:

    - the guy is very unique in his ability to isolated & be able to focus on the differences during an ABX test

    - the files have been analysed & any distortions are down below -90dB

    - he has an amazing ability to switch his listening from one difference aspect in the sound to another

     

    I'm not sure what all this says about our "lying ears" Some things I take away:

    - I intend to download these files if they are still available but I doubt I will be able to tell a difference in an A/B

    - it would be interesting to see if a difference would be noticeable in long term listening (but not necessarily the ability to explicitly identify the difference

    - Doing a successful AB test is very difficult

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Conversely, for the same reason, I would not trust people to discriminate between two samples unless they were *very* closely volume matched.

     

    Sorry if that was confusing, I meant if the test was for ability to discriminate for a factor other than loudness or loudness-based preference.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    I'm not pushing any agenda or endorsing a point of view by publishing this article. I simply think a worthwhile read for all who enjoy this hobby as much as I do.

     

    While you’re not endorsing a point, the author was doing so. After a most circuitous path, which most perceptive, thoughtful, and introspective audiophiles have experienced, his conclusion is: that only when differences are measurable, should a "proper" listening test be conducted.

     

    Wow, what a statement? What does measurable mean? What equipment is used to conduct those measurements? Who is doing the measuring? What parameters are being measured? If there are no measurable differences using whatever techniques and equipment, then a listening test isn't warranted? In support of JRiver, could this be more of the musings of the Consigliere of audio measurements: Archimago? Are we with certitude, to believe that aural differences can always be measured by Archimago, or for that matter anyone else?

     

    Via a well written, cogent description of what many of us have indeed experienced, this poster on JRiver’s forum appears to be providing a backhanded way for JRiver to nullify the cited listening test’s findings (only “4 out of 38” votes for JRMC) and continue the bits-is-bits; they therefore must ALL sound the same, argument. That is, unless differences between bit perfect players can be measured, the players all have the same outcome -- i.e. there is no need to conduct a listening test, "proper" or otherwise! Really, is that the "non-agenda" driven, "worthwhile" conclusion that motivated this submittal?

     

    As a user of JRMC from 16 to now, 21, I’m neither in favor of it sounding the same as other players, nor against that conclusion. My mind is open on the subject. There are just too many variables for me to reach a 100% conclusion. Thus, I believe that there is room for measurements and listening tests and unlike the original JRiver poster; I don’t believe one precludes the other! I frequent forums such as this for healthy, mainly subjective debate. As such, I find dogma unacceptable; especially about such a subjective subject as audio reproduction.

     

    I do however, believe that JRMC has backed themselves in a corner and will never come out, no matter how many listening tests or measurements suggest otherwise. The boxing ring corners have been established and they will never change! As a paying and satisfied customer of JRMC, I find this to be an unfortunate disservice to themselves and to audiophiles in general.

     

    As have many have done when seeking scientific certitude, perhaps when that goal is unattainable, tests have been designed to prove what the tester or funder preferred to prove and/or otherwise. Sometimes we will know that is the case, other times not so much. And so it goes…..

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    The thing is - what is the "accepted" audibility threshold for small distortions?

     

    The page I linked references some studies on that subject, e.g. by the BBC.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    The page I linked references some studies on that subject, e.g. by the BBC.

    Thanks but random jitter is not of that much interest as it is just translates into a fixed level of gaussian noise - data-correlated jitter is of much more interest as it translates into the spikes you see on jitter graphs

     

    I don't know if there a study that shows the audibility level of this form of jitter?

     

    Furthermore, I'm of the opinion that it's mistaken & simplistic to dismiss close-in jitter on the basis that it is close to & therefore will be masked by the main frequency tone. This means that low frequency jitter would be much more important than is thought

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Thanks but random jitter is not of that much interest as it is just translates into a fixed level of gaussian noise - data-correlated jitter is of much more interest as it translates into the spikes you see on jitter graphs

     

    I don't know if there a study that shows the audibility level of this form of jitter?

     

    If it's of any help, jitter is equivalent to frequency modulation.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    If it's of any help, jitter is equivalent to frequency modulation.

     

    That's why it's measured as "phase noise." :)

     

    (I'm not disagreeing with you, I don't have the technical background. Just thought the frequency/phase dichotomy was funny/interesting.)

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    That's actually not correct.

     

     

     

    A/B/X is not the only way to do double-blinded tests, and it may be far from the best way, depending on what you are testing for.

     

    Your entitled to your opinion, you just happen to be wrong.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Wow, what a statement? What does measurable mean? What equipment is used to conduct those measurements? Who is doing the measuring? What parameters are being measured? If there are no measurable differences using whatever techniques and equipment, then a listening test isn't warranted? In support of JRiver, could this be more of the musings of the Consigliere of audio measurements: Archimago? Are we with certitude, to believe that aural differences can always be measured by Archimago, or for that matter anyone else?

     

    Such a grea point, and one that I have been trying to make for a long time. All these people who are so in denial of advances in certain areas just because they can't yet measure them. Yet they have it so backwards. When literally thousands of people around the world report hearing something, why does that not count as a valid data to inspire them to investigate and figure out why and where their measurement techniques fall short?

    The fact is, the engineers designing products have always used a combination of measurement and listening, and those that do end up with the most compelling and successful components. Believe me, it is not an accident.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Your entitled to your opinion, you just happen to be wrong.

     

    Another pseudo scientist who spouts "objectivity and measurements" - but doesn't actually understand testing.

    Actual scientists will agree with Jud. Do a little research yourself and you will find it to be true.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    That's why it's measured as "phase noise." :)

     

    (I'm not disagreeing with you, I don't have the technical background. Just thought the frequency/phase dichotomy was funny/interesting.)

     

    Now that I think about it again, phase modulation would be a more accurate description. That said, frequency and phase modulation are rather similar when the modulating signal is more or less periodic. Shifting the phase of the base signal is the same thing as briefly raising or lowering its frequency.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Such a grea point, and one that I have been trying to make for a long time. All these people who are so in denial of advances in certain areas just because they can't yet measure them. Yet they have it so backwards. When literally thousands of people around the world report hearing something, why does that not count as a valid data to inspire them to investigate and figure out why and where their measurement techniques fall short?

     

    If a lot of people report hearing something, that's certainly cause for an investigation. Now if a multitude of highly accurate measurements fail to turn up any explanation, the logical conclusion at some point must be that the subjective experience is caused by something else.

     

    It is often pointed out that the exact workings of the ear and the auditory processing in the brain are not well understood, and this is true. However, the science behind DACs and amps is very well established. It is extremely unlikely that there are processes going on within these devices of which we are unaware.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    I'm hearing something because someone else said they heard something !

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    If a lot of people report hearing something, that's certainly cause for an investigation. Now if a multitude of highly accurate measurements fail to turn up any explanation, the logical conclusion at some point must be that the subjective experience is caused by something else.

     

    It is often pointed out that the exact workings of the ear and the auditory processing in the brain are not well understood, and this is true. However, the science behind DACs and amps is very well established. It is extremely unlikely that there are processes going on within these devices of which we are unaware.

    You nearly got there but you fell at the last hurdle.

    It's at the interface of what these devices output & auditory perception - that's where it's not well established.

    The illusion that's created is not well understood.

    Are you saying that the device is completely & fully characterised by that a multitude of highly accurate measurements?

     

    If you are can you give the list of measurements needed to fully characterise:

    - an amplifier

    - a DAC

    - a speaker

    - the interfaces between each of these devices

    - a full audio system end to end

     

    Because this would put the issue to bed

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    ....can you give the list of measurements needed to fully characterise:

    - an amplifier

    - a DAC

    - a speaker

    - the interfaces between each of these devices

    - a full audio system end to end

     

    And after doing this characterization, please advise which components in these categories will produce the best recreation of a recorded performance in our homes/listening rooms. If you can do this with an accurate level of confidence amongst the purchasers of such equipment, it would be a godsend; a reason for most equipment manufacturers, forums, and periodicals to go belly up.

     

    In fact, a product could be designed to comply with the science behind your findings and only one product/manufacturer in each category would be needed to satisfy our desire to recreate the best possible reproduction - done, end of story! We can all put this and most every other audio forum to bed. Other than music, what would we have to discuss? Or more importantly, if it wasn’t for our personal, not measurement findings, what would we have to post about? I say “not measurement findings”, because I really don’t frequent this or any other forum for scientific or measurement data. I visit such sites to have a clue about what is piquing everyone’s audio fancy from their listening and use perspective. If there is no scientific or measurable data to defend it and I buy it, the onus is on me, not on someone trying to save me from me!

     

    There will never be an agreement amongst us audiophiles based on measurements, listening or otherwise, that will provide a best to worst list of equipment in each of the available categories. That has been done, is being done and I pay little, to zero attention to same!

     

    Discussing what measurement or listening technique is the de facto standard is a waste of time. Measurements have their place as does listening. One is neither mutually exclusive, nor for me, better than the other.

     

    We're dealing with a hobby that is an emotionally driven one; which, by definition, is a subjective one. Can engineering, science and listening help design great equipment, or answer some of our questions, and resolve some of our issues? Sure, but not all! Perhaps an EKG needs to be put in the measurement mix. Who knows what else?

     

    However, my ears are presently the final arbiter of product purchases, not measurements. In fact, measurements, scientific and engineering information rarely rate in any of my buying decisions. On the other hand, my feelings about what I’m hearing are always at the top of my priority list. And while I was trained in the scientific method and utilized it, and more than fully appreciate it, when it comes to my audio room, or the concert hall, let the scientific method be damned. My main objective is to be whisked into the midst of a performance as fully and emotionally as I can be! If that happens, I really don't care about the science behind it! So to get back to the post that Chris referenced, and the poster’s conclusion amidst all the self-consternation: we must have measurable differences between components, music players etc., before conducting listening tests? No, a thousand times NO!

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    However, the science behind DACs and amps is very well established. It is extremely unlikely that there are processes going on within these devices of which we are unaware.

     

    Except that there ARE many processes going on that are neither well understood nor focused on. Three examples:

    1) Bruno Putzeys' articles and work with high amounts of negative feedback over a very wide bandwidth;

    2) John Swenson's measurements of ground-plane and packet-data noise from USB PHY/MAC circuits making it all the way through to the master clock input of DAC chips regardless of buffers, isolators, and reclocking;

    3) Mike Moffat (and his academia team) upending conventional Parks-McClellan digital filter optimization to put forth a fresh method of upsampling that preserves all original samples.

     

    Each of the above examples were development processes that used a combination of measurement and listening to verify the efficacy of the original hypothesis.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    and after doing this characterization, please advise which components in these categories will produce the best recreation of a recorded performance in our homes/listening rooms. If you can do this with an accurate level of confidence amongst the purchasers of such equipment, it would be a godsend; a reason for most equipment manufacturers, forums, and periodicals to go belly up.

     

    In fact, a product could be designed to comply with the science behind your findings and only one product/manufacturer in each category would be needed to satisfy our desire to recreate the best possible reproduction - done, end of story! We can all put this and most every other audio forum to bed. Other than music, what would we have to discuss? Or more importantly, if it wasn’t for our personal, not measurement findings, what would we have to post about? I say “not measurement findings”, because i really don’t frequent this or any other forum for scientific or measurement data. I visit such sites to have a clue about what is piquing everyone’s audio fancy from their listening and use perspective. If there is no scientific or measurable data to defend it and i buy it, the onus is on me, not on someone trying to save me from me!

     

    There will never be an agreement amongst us audiophiles based on measurements, listening or otherwise, that will provide a best to worst list of equipment in each of the available categories. That has been done, is being done and i pay little, to zero attention to same!

     

    Discussing what measurement or listening technique is the de facto standard is a waste of time. Measurements have their place as does listening. One is neither mutually exclusive, nor for me, better than the other.

     

    We're dealing with a hobby that is an emotionally driven one; which, by definition, is a subjective one. Can engineering, science and listening help design great equipment, or answer some of our questions, and resolve some of our issues? Sure, but not all! Perhaps an ekg needs to be put in the measurement mix. Who knows what else?

     

    However, my ears are presently the final arbiter of product purchases, not measurements. In fact, measurements, scientific and engineering information rarely rate in any of my buying decisions. On the other hand, my feelings about what i’m hearing are always at the top of my priority list. And while i was trained in the scientific method and utilized it, and more than fully appreciate it, when it comes to my audio room, or the concert hall, let the scientific method be damned. My main objective is to be whisked into the midst of a performance as fully and emotionally as i can be! If that happens, i really don't care about the science behind it! So to get back to the post that chris referenced, and the poster’s conclusion amidst all the self-consternation: We must have measurable differences between components, music players etc., before conducting listening tests? No, a thousand times no!

     

    Great post Mike!! :)

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    3) Mike Moffat (and his academia team) upending conventional Parks-McClellan digital filter optimization to put forth a fresh method of upsampling that preserves all original samples.

     

    Each of the above examples were development processes that used a combination of measurement and listening to verify the efficacy of the original hypothesis.

     

    While I agree with your general line of thought, Mike Moffat implemented that filtering decades ago in the Theta products. Silicon available these days (and perhaps design tools as well?) no doubt allows for better realization than was possible then. For example, I'm reasonably sure the old Theta DACs didn't have 15,000 taps.

     

    I also think there are at least a couple of other folks with ideas along generally similar lines.

     

    The foregoing may sound as if I feel Mike Moffat's work is less special than I actually do. I'm very excited to hear an Yggdrasil.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    I'm very excited to hear an Yggdrasil.

     

    Yes, me too. I may have to buy one to hear it. :)

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Thanks for posting this. Being new to the "audiophile" world, I was perplexed by this article when I read it last night:

     

     

    Two USB Cables | The Absolute Sound

     

     

    Specificaly, how can the physical properties/quality of a usb cable specifically target "the middle range of the musical spectrum" of a digital signal? How can the "analog" electrical pulse that travels down a USB cable be effected in such a way (and it is of course influenced by its physical environment as all physical/"analog" signals are) that specifically targets digital information only in "the middle range of the musical spectrum", and in such a way that the buffer/DAC is "influenced" by said information change and passes it on down the chain? It defies any explanation that I am aware of - and I suspect it is all in the reviewers head.

     

     

    Again, thanks for the perspective this essay provides...

     

    Ringing and eddy currents cause electrical distortions that affect the amplifier that can heard if you are listening on good equipment. much has been learned about electrical distortion and its effects. Poor quality cable, crappy solder and poorly soldered joints, oxide buildup on connectors cause measure able ringing and electrical noise.

     

     

    Of course much of what read in this thread seems pretty self congratulatory on how you guys can't hear anything. So yea, waste of typing here, I know.

     

    Just do this, listen to your favorite music for a while. Then disconnect and reconnect every cable on your system and then listen again. Respond back on whether it sounds different or not.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    Ringing and eddy currents cause electrical distortions that affect the amplifier that can heard if you are listening on good equipment. much has been learned about electrical distortion and its effects. Poor quality cable, crappy solder and poorly soldered joints, oxide buildup on connectors cause measure able ringing and electrical noise.

     

     

    Of course much of what read in this thread seems pretty self congratulatory on how you guys can't hear anything. So yea, waste of typing here, I know.

     

    Just do this, listen to your favorite music for a while. Then disconnect and reconnect every cable on your system and then listen again. Respond back on whether it sounds different or not.

     

    If the cables have been plugged in a while, of course it will sound different. The contacts corrode slightly over time, and reseating them produces a better contact. There is nothing controversial about that. Now, Quantum Tunneling cables, those are much more debatable. ;)

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites

    If the cables have been plugged in a while, of course it will sound different. The contacts corrode slightly over time, and reseating them produces a better contact. There is nothing controversial about that.

     

    This is true for analogue cables, or at least it can be if corrosion is bad enough (gold-plated contacts in a typical home environment don't corrode much). For digital cables, if the bits get through unmolested, they sound the same even if the contacts are not perfect. There is nothing controversial about that.

    Share this comment


    Link to comment
    Share on other sites




    Guest
    This is now closed for further comments




×
×
  • Create New...